Journal of Depression And Therapy

Journal of Depression and Therapy

Journal of Depression and Therapy

Current Issue Volume No: 1 Issue No: 3

Research Article Open Access Available online freely Peer Reviewed Citation

A Randomized Controlled Biofeedback Intervention Study On Heart Rate Variability In Unemployed Subjects

1Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Dresden, Germany University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus Dresden, Technische Universitaet Dresden, Fetscherstr.

2Department of Psychology, Technische Universitaet Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Abstract

Background:

Unemployment is a chronic stressor which is associated with higher levels of impaired physical and mental wellbeing, e.g. reduced heart rate variability (HRV) and decline in self-esteem and self-efficacy. The present study investigated the effectivity of a HRV-biofeedback-training in increasing HRV and general self-efficacy.

Method:

A total of n = 22 unemployed individuals with adjustment disorder participated in this study (14 females, mean ages 42.53 ± 8.88 years). All participants performed randomized the “stress tests” for heart rate (HR) and HRV assessment: timed breathing, d2-attention-stress-test and math-test drawn from the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). Four sessions HRV-biofeedback à 20 min within 2 weeks were administered. HR and HRV were assessed before and after biofeedback training. ECG preprocessing and analysis were performed with the Stressball software program (BioSign GmbH, Ottenhofen, Germany). The RMSSD time domain measure was calculated as HRV index. Clinical outcome measures were the ADNM total score and the general self-efficacy scale (GSE).

Results:

Participants were well matched in terms of demographic and clinical characteristics. ANOVA didn’t demonstrate significant time x group interaction effects neither for HRV parameters (HR, RMSSD) nor for psychological parameters (ADNM, GSE).

Conclusion:

Our findings do not indicate that HRV-biofeedback can increase HRV nor general self-efficacy in unemployed subjects with adjustment disorder. Implications for future studies are discussed.

Author Contributions
Received 14 Oct 2016; Accepted 18 Dec 2016; Published 24 Mar 2017;

Academic Editor: FARUK UGUZ, Necmettin Erbakan University, Meram Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry

Checked for plagiarism: Yes

Review by: Single-blind

Copyright ©  2017 Katja Petrowski, et al.

License
Creative Commons License     This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Competing interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Citation:

Katja Petrowski, Katharina Wendt, Susann Wichmann, Martin Siepmann (2017) A Randomized Controlled Biofeedback Intervention Study On Heart Rate Variability In Unemployed Subjects. Journal of Depression and Therapy - 1(3):18-28. https://doi.org/10.14302/issn.2476-1710.jdt-16-1332

Download as RIS, BibTeX, Text (Include abstract )

DOI 10.14302/issn.2476-1710.jdt-16-1332

Introduction

Unemployment is a growing problem. The unemployment quota ranged at 18.3% at the beginning of this study in the year 2005 1. It is well known that unemployment brings to bear negative effects on the psychological 2, 3, 4, 5 and physical state of health of those affected 6, 7, 8, e. g. decline in self-esteem 9, increased prevalence of depression 10 and increased risk of cardiovascular disease 11 were observed. Even higher mortality rates 12, 13, 14, 15, 16 and suicidal tendencies 14, 16 have been reported. Therefore it is even more important to research the influencing factors causing these negative results as well as evaluating intervention strategies to minimize these negative effects.

Unemployment is a chronic stressor potentially disturbing autonomic balance when adjustment to job loss fails. Heart rate variability (HRV) is an established marker of autonomic flexibility, or in the case of depressed HRV, of autonomic inflexibility in response to changing external and internal stressors 17. Jandackova, Paulik, and Steptoe (2012) demonstrated decreased HRV parameters in unemployed subjects 18. Unemployment may be associated with factors that negatively influence HRV parameters, such as smoking, drinking, physical inactivity and high body mass index that could account for the increased prevalence of cardiovascular diseases 18. HRV is defined as the beat to beat variability of consecutive heartbeats 19. It is regulated by the balanced interaction of the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous system. Parasympathetic activity is reflected in adjustments of the heart rate (HR) in response to changes in blood pressure or respiratory status; the sympathetic tone controls changes in HR in response to physical or mental stress 20. In medical science and practice, HRV has established itself as an independent predictor for an increased risk of mortality after a myocardial attack 21. In clinical psychological research, decreased HRV parameters were associated with negative affectivity, especially with anxiety 22 and anger 23 and with mental disorders such as depression 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32 and anxiety disorders 22, 33.

Up to now, there is little evidence for interventions providing support to unemployed subjects in reducing distress 34 and existing studies focused primarily on re-employment. Interventions supporting adjustment to job loss could also focus the associated negative health effects, e.g. the previous finding of reduced HRV 18. Studies documented a positive effect of HRV-biofeedback training on HRV 35, 36, 37. Biofeedback is a scientifically based method in which physiological bodily processes, which normally are not perceived consciously, are made visible and, thus, individually amenable to influence. Physiological signals (e.g. heart rate) are registered, amplified, and visually or acoustically confirmed. Thus, seemingly unable to be influenced bodily functions can be controlled via biofeedback processes 38. Recent studies further indicate that HRV biofeedback is effective in the treatment of depression 39, 40, 41, posttraumatic stress disorders 42, 43, sleeping disorders 43, stress disorders/stress reactions 44, 45, 46, anxiety disorders 47, the attention-deficit-hyperactivity-syndrome 48, fibromyalgia 39, and bronchial asthma 49, 50.

The processes by which HRV biofeedback improves health and well-being are not yet understand. It is suggested that HRV biofeedback produces resonance condition between HR, breathing rate and baroreceptor activity allowing for maximized amplitudes of the HRV 37. Resonance frequency differs interindividually but on average it is 0.1 Hz, corresponding to 6 breathes per minute 37. Further, biofeedback improves interoceptive awareness of any senses stimulated within the body 51. With the help of biofeedback, participants learn to perceive and control somatic functions that actually are not perceived consciously. Thus biofeedback may help to improve well-being by increasing self-efficacy and the sense of control. This idea agrees with Turner, Kessler, and House (1991) who recommended providing interventions to unemployed subjects that bolster self-efficacy 52. Work is one major source of self-efficacy that declines following job loss and is regained with re-employment 9. Further self-efficacy is crucial in motivating job-seeking behavior since low self-esteem is associated with inhibition of job-seeking activities 53.

The aim of the present study is to evaluate the effectivity of HRV-biofeedback in normalizing HRV (increased RMSSD, decreased mean HR) and increasing ratings on self-efficacy in a sample of unemployed subjects.

Methods

Study Participants

The data were collected from April 2005 to October 2007. The examined group (n = 22) consisted of unemployed individuals from the vicinity of Dresden. To be eligible, participants needed to be aged between 18 and 60 years, fluent in German, unemployed for at least six months and they needed to meet the criteria for an adjustment disorder following unemployment. Exclusion criteria included alcohol and substance abuse, pulmonary (e. g. chronic obstructive or restrictive functioning disease), cardiac (e. g. arrhythmia, heart insufficiency, cardiac valve disease), endocrinological (e.g. thyroid conditions, diabetes mellitus) or neurological diseases (e.g. polyneuropathy), arterial hypertony, malnutrition, underweight or the intake of heart frequency modulating medications, especially beta blockers, tricyclic antidepressants or thyroid hormones. Participants were randomly assigned to either the biofeedback treatment group (n = 11) or the waiting control group (n = 11). All the study participants provided written informed consent. The study protocol was approved by the local Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Technical University of Dresden, Germany (No# EK148082006).

Procedures

Stress Tests.

For the determination of the HRV, the so-called “stress tests“ were carried out at a two-week-interval at two measuring times. These consisted of five parts, for which the RMSSD ms as well as the mean heart frequency (bpmin) were calculated, respectively. The stress tests were the following: (1) 1-minute timed breathing interval (deep respiration), (2) 3-minute measurement under stress condition (concentration), (3) 1-minute timed breathing interval (deep respiration), (4) 3-minute measurement under stress condition (math task), (5) 1-minute timed breathing interval (deep respiration).

First of all, the participant was instructed to inhale and exhale as deeply as possible in the given breathing rhythm of six breaths per minute. This was carried out by way of a visual beam which was moved up and down, thus instructing the participant about the inspiration and the expiration phase. Paced breathing with six breaths per minute allows for maximum amplitudes of the HRV 20.

The following tasks were used for stress induction: The d2-attention-stress-test (d2) 54 is a performance test (concentration test) which serves to record the general requirements for obtaining different performances (e.g. logical thinking, spatial imagination). This measure calls for a high degree of attention/concentration, however no special aptitudes. It is a widely applicable measure, whereby the necessary attention/concentration was used as stress-inducing component in the framework of this study. The d2 is a homogenous test with 14 test lines containing 47 letters each (d’s and p’s with 1 to 4 strokes each). The test participants’ task was to score out all d‘s with two strokes at an allotted time of 20 seconds per line, respectively. The tests consist of working through the standardized instruction as quickly and as accurately as possible. As there was a stress period of three minutes scheduled for this study, the work process went through line nine.

The math test also used for stress induction is an element drawn from the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) 55. The test participants were instructed to count backwards from the figure 2046 in steps of 17 and to announce the respective results out loud. There was no reaction to any of this from the study coordinator to indicate the correctness of a result, compelling the test participant to automatically continue with the task. If a result was incorrect, the study participant had to begin anew at 2046. The TSST is a well-established tool for stress induction.

Biofeedback.

Participants in the biofeedback group were trained for four sessions within two weeks, at approximately at the same time of day for each subject. Inter-individual noise was reduced by instructing participants to abstain from food and drink consumption preceding testing 56. Further participants confirmed that they had not drank alcohol or caffeine-containing beverages or smoked during the previous three hours.

The Stressball biofeedback system (BioSign, Ottenhofen, Germany) was used. Participants were seated in a comfortable chair and instructed to metronome-controlled breathing with six deep breathes per minute (0.1 Hz) allowing for maximized amplitudes of HRV 37. A balloon on a computer screen that moved up for inspiration and down for expiration was used for pacing respiration. The parameter being displayed was the overall HRV. Each session was 20 minutes long. Additionally, participants of the biofeedback group were asked to perform a breathing exercise at home three times the day lasting 5 to 10 minutes. The breathing exercise guided participants to breathe with the aforementioned metronome of 0.1 Hz with the help of a rising and falling tone indicating inspiration and expiration. This exercise was introduced at the first measuring time. The participants received an audio disc and an instruction paper to facilitate at-home practicing as well as a protocol to minute all exercises. Both the biofeedback and waiting control group were tested again for HRV assessment with the stress-tests after cessation of the biofeedback training.

Measures

Clinical Assessments

The individual’s ability to adjust to job loss was evaluated by assessing the manifestation of an adjustment disorder using the 29-item-self-report questionnaire adjustment disorder new module (ADNM) which consists of two parts 57. In part one, the respondent is asked to report stressful life events out of seven categories (severe illness, family conflicts, divorce/separation, conflicts with colleagues/at work, illness of relative, financial problems, leaving working life) and to decide which one was the most distressing. The second part involves the diagnostic criteria for an adjustment disorder belonging to the core symptoms of intrusion, avoidance and failure to adapt and the subtype criteria (anxiety, depressed mood, impulse disturbance). Part two is rated on a 4-point Likert-scale. Two out of three of the core anxiety or depressive symptoms have to be present for subtype manifestation.

To differentiate psychological and physical impairment the Complaint List (BL) 58 was used which consists of 24 items with a four-point rating scale (range 0-72). Severity of depressive symptoms was self-evaluated using the Beck-Depression-Inventory II (BDI-II) 59, 60. The respondent is asked to choose the most appropriate alternative out of 21 items matching the DSM-IV major depression criteria that best describes the way he or she felt during the past two weeks (range 0-63). Subjects’ sense of personal competence to deal effectively with stressful situations was evaluated with the general self-efficacy scale (GSE) 61. This self-report instrument contains 10 items which are rated on a four-point Likert scale (range 10-40).

Heart Rate Variability

The Stressball software program (BioSign GmbH, Ottenhofen, Germany) was used for the computer-aided recording of the HRV. The recording of the pulse-wave was carried out by an infra-red sensor positioned on the earlobe. The HRV-Scanner software program (BioSign GmbH, Ottenhofen, Germany) was employed for the evaluation of the data. It first of all served the automatic determination of the HR by way of the pulse wave. This was followed by the automatically recorded heartbeats were checked manually for all the individual measurements; artifacts as well as possible extra systoles were manually filtered out. The adaptation of the data was carried out according to extensive product instruction by the author. Maximally two extra systoles or artifacts each were accepted per measurement. Hence, it was permissible to delete two RR-intervals. The subsequent calculation of the parameters then took place automatically with the above mentioned software program. For HRV assessment, the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) as an established time-domain parameter of HRV was calculated 62. The RMSSD parameter is a robust index of high-frequency variations in HR due to parasympathetic control of HR 19. The most recent studies, among them that of Nussovitch et al. (2011), were able to show that the RMSSD reliably represented the HRV even in short (1 min) and ultra-short (10 sec) recordings 63.

Statistical Analysis

Group differences in sociodemographic and clinical characteristics were tested using univariate ANOVA for continuous variables and chi-squared test for dichotomous variables. The effect of biofeedback on HRV (mean HR, RMSSD) was examined using repeated measures ANOVA with group 2 as between-subject factor and stress test condition 3 and measuring time 2 as within-subject factors. ANOVA results were corrected by Greenhouse-Geisser whenever necessary. Data analyses were performed using SPSS v. 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

The group’s characteristics are described in Table 1. Groups were well-matched on sociodemographic and clinical variables.

Table 1. Characteristics of the total sample. Mean (SD) are listed except where noted
  Biofeedback group(n = 11) ControlGroup (n = 11) Test statistic p
Age (years) 41.09 (7.77) 44.36 (8.73) F1;21 = 0.863 .364
Females, n (%) 7 (63.6) 7 (63.6) Χ² = 0.00 .670
Body mass index 29.55 (7.49) 26.65 (6.08) F1;21 = 0.994 .331
ADNM 13.18 (1.94) 13.11 (1.69) F1;19 = 0.007 .933
BDI 18.91 (6.28) 19.73 (6.64) F1;21 = 0.088 .770
CL 38.50 (9.77) 42.90 (10.05) F1;19 = 0.986 .334

Note. ADNM = adjustment disorder new module sum score; BDI = Beck-Depression-Inventory sum score; CL = Complaint List sum score.

Means and standard deviations for each HRV parameter are displayed in Table 2 as a function of stress test condition and measuring time. ANOVA revealed a main effect of stress test condition for the RMSSD (F1.471;29.425 = 39.707, p < 0.001, ç2 = 0.665) and mean HR (F1.897;37.948 = 42.386, p < 0.001, ç2 = 0.679). Pair-wise comparisons revealed significant differences for all stress test conditions (p ≤ 0.021), except for comparing mean HR during the d2-attention-stress-test and math-test (p = 1.000). As expected, increased RMSSD was observed during timed breathing with 0.1 HZ. ANOVA didn’t demonstrate a main effect of group neither for the RMSSD (F1;20 = 2.400, p = 0.137, ç2 = 0.107) nor for mean HR (F1;20 = 0.011, p = 0.919, ç2 = 0.001) as well as no interaction effect between study group and stress test condition.

Regarding ADNM sum score, ANOVA failed to reveal a main effect of time (F1;18 = 1.029, p = .324, ç2= 0.054) and of group (F1;18 = 1.121, p = .304, ç2 = 0.059) or time x group interaction effect (F1;18 = 2.085, p = .166, ç2= 0.104). With regards to the GSE sum score ANOVA didn’t demonstrate a main effect of time (F1;20 = 1.547, p = .228, ç2= 0.072) or group (F1;20 = 1.083, p = .310, ç2 = 0.051) nor time x group interaction effect (F1;20 = 2.614, p = .122, ç2= 0.116).

Table 2. HRV parameters and psychological assessment as a function of study group and stress test condition. Mean (SD) are listed
  Biofeedback-group(n = 11) Control group(n = 11)
Pre Post Pre Post
HRV parameters
Mean HR Timed breathing 75.51 (9.34) 74.28 (11.22) 73.76 (10.54) 76.44 (9.80)
D2-attention-stress-test 84.88 (9.72) 79.75 (11.47) 81.17 (10.46) 82.07 (9.59)
Math-test 85.27 (8.33) 80.11 (13.16) 81.29 (9.89) 82.60 (9.19)
RMSSD Timed breathing 1.66 (0.32) 1.69 (0.29) 1.56 (0.23) 1.47 (0.24)
D2-attention-stress-test 1.36 (0.21) 1.42 (0.18) 1.31 (0.13) 1.27 (0.14)
Math-test 1.42 (0.16) 1.46 (0.24) 1.38 (0.16) 1.31 (0.17)
Psychological assessment      
ADNM   13.18 (1.94) 11.27 (4.13) 13.11 (1.69) 13.44 (2.56)
SWE   21.36 (4.78) 23.45 (5.48) 24.73 (5.76) 24.45 (4.72)

Note. Pre = before the biofeedback training; Post = after the biofeedback training; HR = heart rate; RMSSD = square root of successive R-R interval differences. RMSSD was log10 transformed.

Discussion

This study investigated the effectivity of heart rate variability (HRV)-biofeedback in improving HRV and general self-efficacy in unemployed subjects. To date, there is little research addressing intervention strategies to minimize negative effects on wellbeing following job loss. Previous studies demonstrated decreased HRV 18 and impaired general self-efficacy in unemployed subjects 9. HRV-biofeedback is suggested to improve HRV as well as self-efficacy 51. In this study, we found no significant effect of HRV-biofeedback on HRV and general self-efficacy.

Our results are in contrast to those of studies that reported increased HRV parameters following HRV-biofeedback-training 35, 36. Our findings didn’t show significant alterations in HRV parameters following HRV-Biofeedback. The samples of Bacior et al. (2004) as well as Larsen et al. (2004) included patients with chronic heart failure who showed decreased HRV. Since this study didn’t investigate a reference population, we couldn’t state if HRV parameters were decreased in our sample compared to healthy control participants. To reliably evaluate HRV parameters in reference to normal ranges, this study should have included a healthy control group that should have passed the stress tests as well to serve as a reference population. According to a study by Agelink et al. (2001) both male and female participants of our study showed RMSSD parameters that are in line with established standard values 64. This reduces the plasticity of improving initial RMSSD. Further, our data failed to show significant alterations regarding self-efficacy and the ADNM sum-score following HRV-biofeedback. On a descriptive level, the biofeedback group was able to improve their RMSSD and to decrease their mean HR in all stress-test-conditions. Failure to reach significance may result from the small sample size or short intervention time.

HRV assessment was largely performed in accordance with the recommendations of the Task Force 19. Two variations were made. First, HR was derived by way of the pulse wave, and not via recommended ECG recording. But this method appears just as well reliable and not more interference-prone like ECG recording. Second, the Task Force recommends 5 minutes duration of ECG recording for short-term HRV assessment. In the present study, stress conditions lasted only 3 minutes and paced breathing was performed for 1 minute. ECG analysis was restricted to RMSSD time domain measure, and previous findings report on the reliability of the RMSSD for HRV assessment from recording of 1 minute and even of 10 seconds 63. Hence the present recording duration appears to be sufficiently reliable.

There are some limitations that should be noted. First, the sample size was small. Another limitation of this study is that no baseline HRV resting condition had been included. Paced breathing can induce stress and hence doesn’t serve well as resting condition. Future studies should include a resting session to compare resting and stressful conditions in HRV assessment. Future studies should also include a real control group without adjustment disorder and a reference population for the establishment of normal ranges of HRV parameters. Moreover, the present study included only a short interval of two weeks to observe changes in HRV. This may be too short to detect adaptation in the autonomous nervous system. Future studies should extend the intervention interval.

In conclusion, we found no significant effect of a 2-week HRV-biofeedback-training. Thus, we didn’t find evidence to support HRV-biofeedback-training in the present form to be an efficacious intervention to increase HRV and self-efficacy. Future studies should include a control group and extend the intervention interval.

Conflict of Interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsiniki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Acknowledgements

The present work was part of a graduate thesis.

References

  1. 1.Bundesagentur für Arbeit. (2006) . Arbeitsmarkt 2005. Amtliche Nachrichten der Bundesagentur für Arbeit 54. Jahrgang .
  1. 2.Berth H, Foerster P, Braehler E. (2003) . Gesundheitsfolgen von Arbeitslosigkeit und Arbeitsplatzunsicherheit bei jungen Erwachsenen. Gesundheitswesen 65, 555-560.
  1. 3.Kokko K, Pulkkinen L, Puustinen M. (2000) Selection into long-term unemployment and its psychological consequences. , International Journal of Behavioral Development 24(3), 310-320.
  1. 4.Lauenroth N, &Swart E. (2004) . Perspektivlosigkeit ist meine Krankheit – Subjektive Empfindungen von Langzeitarbeitslosen. Gesundheitswesen 66(11), 765-769.
  1. 5.A H Winefield, Tiggemann M. (1990) Employment status and psychological well-being: A longitudinal study. , Journal of Applied Psychology 75(4), 455-459.
  1. 6.D G Cook, M J Bartley, R O Cummins, A G Shaper. (1982) . Health of unemployed middle-aged men in Great Britain. Lancet 319(8284), 1290-1294.
  1. 7.W T Gallo, E H Bradley, J A Dubin, R N Jones, T A Falba et al. (2006) The persistence of depressive symptoms in older workers who experience involuntary job loss: Results from the health and retirement survey. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 61(4), 221-228.
  1. 8.Weber A, Hoerman G, Heipertz W. (2007) . Arbeitslosigkeit und Gesundheit aus sozialmedizinischer Sicht. Deutsches Aerzteblatt, Jg. 104(Heft 43) 2957-2962.
  1. 9.Warr P, Jackson P. (1985) Factors influencing the psychological impact of prolonged unemployment and of re-employment. , Psychological Medicine 15(04), 795-807.
  1. 10.M A Dew, E J Bromet, Penkower L. (1992) Mental health effects of job loss in women. , Psychological Medicine 22(3), 751-764.
  1. 11.W T Gallo, Bradley A E H, T A Falba, J A Dubin, L D Cramer et al. (2004) Involuntary job loss as a risk factor for subsequent myocardial infarction and stroke: Findings from the Health and Retirement Survey. , American Journal of Industrial Medicine 416, 408-416.
  1. 12.M H Brenner. (1987) Economic change, alcohol consumption and heart disease mortality in nine industrialized countries. , Social Science & Medicine 25(2), 119-132.
  1. 13.Grobe T, Doerning H, Schwartz F. (1999) . Auswertungen der GEK Gesunheitsberichterstattung Schwerpunkt Arbeitslosigkeit und Gesundheit. GEK Gmünder ErsatzKasse (Eds.), Schwäbisch Gmünd .
  1. 14.Kurella S. (1992) Arbeitslosigkeit und Gesundheit Literaturstudie für die Jahre 1985-1991. Wissenschaftszentrum Berlin für Sozialforschung. , Berlin
  1. 15.P T Martikainen. (1990) Unemployment and mortality among Finnish men,1981-5. , British Medical Journal 301(6749), 407-411.
  1. 16.Voss M, Nylén L, Floderus B, Diderichsen F, P D Terry. (2004) Unemployment and early cause-specific mortality: A study based on the Swedish twin registry. , American Journal of Public Health 94(12), 2155-2161.
  1. 17.J F Thayer, J F Brosschot. (2005) Psychosomatics and psychopathlogy: Looking up and down from the brain. , Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(10), 1050-1058.
  1. 18.V K Jandackova, Paulik K, Steptoe A. (2012) The impact of unemployment on heart rate variability: The evidence from the Czech Republic. , Biological Psychology 91, 238-244.
  1. 19.Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology,1996. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. , Circulation 93(5), 1043-1065.
  1. 20.Ziemssen T, Suess M, Reichmann H. (2002) . Die kardiovaskuläre Funktionsdiagnostik des autonomen Nervensystems – eine Einführung. Das Neurophysiologie-Labor 24, 57-77.
  1. 21.Malik M. (1998) Heart rate variability. , Current Opinion in Cardiology 13(1), 36-44.
  1. 22.B H Friedman, J F Thayer. (1998) Autonomic balance revisited: Panic anxiety and heart rate variability. , Journal of Psychosomatic Research 44(1), 133-151.
  1. 23.M E Bleil, P J Gianaros, J R, J D Flory, S B Manuck. (2008) Trait negative affect: Toward an integrated model of understanding psychological risk for impairment in cardiac autonomic function. , Psychosomatic Medicine 70(3), 328-337.
  1. 24.R M Carney, M W Rich, TeVelde A, Saini J, Clark K et al. (1988) The relationship between heart rate, heart rate variability and depression in patients with coronary artery disease. , Journal of Psychosomatic Research 32(2), 159-164.
  1. 25.R M Carney, R D Saunders, K E Freedland, Stein P, M W Rich et al. (1995) Association of depression with reduced heart rate variability in coronary artery disease. , American Journal of Cardiology 76(8), 562-564.
  1. 26.R M Carney, J A Blumenthal, P K Stein, Watkins L, Catellier D et al. (2001) Depression, heart rate variability, and acute myocardial infarction. , Circulation 104, 2024-2028.
  1. 27.Horsten M, Ericson M, Perski A, Wamala S, Schenck-Gustafsson K et al. (1999) Psychosocial factors and heart rate variability in healthy women. , Psychosomatic Medicine 61(1), 49-57.
  1. 28.J W Hughes, C M Stoney. (2000) Depressed mood is related to high-frequency heart rate variability during stressors. , Psychosomatic Medicine 62(6), 796-803.
  1. 29.Krittayaphong R, W E Cascio, K C Light, Sheffield D, R N Golden et al. (1997) Heart rate variability in patients with coronary artery disease: Differences in patients with higher and lower depression scores. , Psychosomatic Medicine 59(3), 231-235.
  1. 30.Mueck-Weymann M, Moesler T, Joraschky P, Rebensburg M, M W Agelink. (2002) Depression modulates autonomic cardiac control: A physiopsychological pathway linking depression and mortality?. , German Journal of Psychiatry 5, 67-69.
  1. 31.Nahshoni E, Aravot D, Aizenberg D, Sigler M, Zalsman G et al. (2004) Heart rate variability in patients with major depression. , Psychosomatics 45(2), 129-134.
  1. 32.Udupa K, T N Sathyaprabha, Thirthalli J, K R, G S Lavekar et al. (1994) Alteration of cardiac autonomic functions in patients with major depression: A study using heart rate variability measures. , Journal of Affective Disorders,100(1-3) 137-141.
  1. 33.Pittig A, J, Lam C W R, M G Craske. (2013) Heart rate and heart rate variability in panic, social anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and generalized anxiety disorders at baseline and in response to relaxation and hyperventilation. , International Journal of Psychophysiology 87(1), 10-1016.
  1. 34.Audhoe S S, Hoving J L, Sluiter J K, MHW Frings-Dresen. (2010) Vocational interventions for unemployed: Effects on work participation and mental distress. A systematic review. , Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation 20(1), 1-13.
  1. 35.Bacior B, Kubinyi A, Klecha A, Styczkiewicz K, Olszanecka A et al. (2004) Heart rate variability and blood pressure variability in patients with heart failure undergoing cardiac rehabilitation. Kardiologia. Polska,61(Suppl2) 82-8.
  1. 36.A I Larsen, Gjesdal K, Hall C, Aukrust P, Aarsland T et al. (2004) Effect of exercise training in patients with heart failure: A pilot study on autonomic balance assessed by heart rate variability. , European Journal of Preventive Cardiology 2, 162-167.
  1. 37.E G Vaschillo, Vaschillo B, P M Lehrer. (2006) Characteristics of resonance in heart rate variability stimulated by biofeedback. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 31(2), 129-142.
  1. 38.Mueck-Weymann M. (2007) Herzratenvariabilitäts-Biofeedback. Forum Stressmedizin (I),1-7. Retrieved from http://www.stresspilot.biz/download/Herzratenvariabilitaetsfeedback_Mueck- Weymann_forumstressmedizin_07.pdf.
  1. 39.A L Hasset, D C Radvanski, E G Vaschillo, Vaschillo B, L H Sigal et al. (2007) A pilot study of the efficacy of heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback in patients with fibromyalgia. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 32(1), 1-10.
  1. 40.M K Karavidas, P M Lehrer, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Marin H et al. (2007) Preliminary results of an open label study of heart rate variability biofeedback for treatment of major depression. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 32(1), 10-1007.
  1. 41.Siepmann M, Aykac V, Unterdoerfer J, Petrowski K, Mueck-Weymann M. (2008) A pilot study on the effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with depression and in healthy subjects. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 33(4), 195-201.
  1. 42.Tan G, T K Dao, Farmer L, R J Sutherland, Gevirtz R. (2011) Heart rate variability (HRV) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): A pilot study. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 36(1), 27-35.
  1. 43.T L Zucker, K W Samuelson, Muench F, M A Greenberg, Gevirtz R. (2009) The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: A pilot study. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 34(2), 135-143.
  1. 44.Loellgen D, Mueck-Weymann M, Beise R. (2009) Herzratenvariabilitäts-Biofeedback in der betrieblichen Gesundheitsförderung - Eine Pilotstudie. Forum Stressmedizin (I),1-11. Retrieved from http://www.stresspilot.biz/download/HRVbiofeedback_im_BGM_Loellgen_forumstressmedizin_09.pdf.
  1. 45.Mueck-Weymann M, Beise R. (2005) Herzkohärenztraining - eine moderne Form der Stressbewältigung. Forum Stressmedizin (I),1-5. Retrieved from http://www.strophantus.de/mediapool/59/596780/data/Herzkohaer...pdf.
  1. 46.Sherlin L, Muench F, Wyckoff S. (2010) Respiratory sinus arrhythmia feedback in a stressed population exposed to a brief stressor demonstrated by quantitative EEG and sLORETA. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 35(3), 219-228.
  1. 47.Sherlin L, Gevirtz R, Wyckoff S, Muench F. (2009) Effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback versus passive biofeedback control. , International Journal of Stress Management 16(3), 233-248.
  1. 48.Eisenberg J, Ben-Daniel N, Mei-Tal G, Wertmann E. (2004) An automomic nervous system biofeedback modality for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder – an open pilot study. , Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences 41, 45-53.
  1. 49.Lehrer P, Smetankin A, Potapova T. (2000) Respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback therapy for asthma: A report of 20 unmedicated pediatric cases using smetanking method. , Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 25(3), 193-200.
  1. 50.P M Lehrer, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Lu S-E, D L Eckberg et al. (2003) Heart rate variability biofeedback increases baroreflex gain and peak expiratory flow. , Psychosomatic Medicine 65(5), 796-805.
  1. 51.Mueck-Weymann M, Einsle F. (2005) . , Biofeedback.In V Koellner & M Broda(Eds.),PraktischeVerhaltensmedizin(1sted.,pp.69-75).Stuttgart:Thieme
  1. 52.Turner J B, Kessler R C, House J S. (1991) Factors facilitating adjustment to unemployment: Implications for intervention. , American Journal of Community Psychology 19(4), 521-542.
  1. 53.N T Feather, G E O’Brien. (1986) A longitudinal study of the effects of employment and unemployment on school-leavers. , Journal of Occupational Psychology 59, 121-144.
  1. 54.Brickenkamp R. (1962) Konzentrations-Belastungs-Test d2. , Göttingen: Hogrefe
  1. 55.Kirschbaum C, K M Pirke, D H Hellhammer. (1993) The trier social stress test – a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. , Neuropsychobiology,28(1-2) 76-81.
  1. 56.D S Quintana, Heathers J A J. (2014) Considerations in the assessment of heart rate variability in biobehavioral research. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 1-10.
  1. 57.Einsle F, Maercker A. (2004) Fragebogen Adjustment Disorder – New Module (ADNM). Universität Zürich, Psychologisches Institut, Psychopathologie und Klinische Intervention. Retrieved from http://www.psychologie.uzh.ch/fachrichtungen/psypath/ForschungTools/Fragebogen.html.
  1. 58.D von Zerssen. (1976) . , Die Beschwerdenliste, Manual. Weinheim: Beltz Test
  1. 59.Beck A, Steer R. (1998) Depression Inventory II – Manual San Antonio,TX:Psychological Cooperation.
  1. 60.Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kuehner C. (2006) Beck Depressionsinventar II.Frankfurt/Main:Harcourt.
  1. 61.Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M. (1999) . Skalen zur Erfassung von Lehrer- und Schülermerkmalen. Dokumentation der psychometrischen Verfahren im Rahmen der Wissenschaftlichen Begleitung des Modellversuchs Selbstwirksame Schulen , Berlin: Freie Universität .
  1. 62.Baron R, Wasner G. (2005) . , Herzfrequenzvariabilität.In H Buchner & J Noth (Eds.), Evozierte Potenziale, Neurovegetative Diagnostik, Okulographie: Methodik und klinische Anwendung(1sted.,pp.176-179).Stuttgart:Thieme
  1. 63.Nussinovitch U, K P Elishkevitz, Katz K, Nussinovitch M, Segev S et al. (2011) Reliability of ultra-short ECG indices for heart rate variability. , Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology 16(2), 117-122.
  1. 64.M W Agelink, Malessa R, Baumann B, Majewski T, Akila F et al. (2001) Standardized tests of heart rate variability: Normal ranges obtained from 309 healthy humans, and effects of age, gender, and heart rate. , Clinical Autonomic Research 11(2), 99-108.